Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 March 2021
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Trajectories of Brain Abnormalities in Early Schizophrenia View all 14 articles

White Matter Correlates of Theory of Mind in Patients With First-Episode Psychosis

  • 1Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Seoul National University College of Natural Science, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Biomedical Research Institute, Pusan National University Yangsan Hospital, Yangsan, South Korea
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Pusan National University Yangsan Hospital, Yangsan, South Korea

Deficits in theory of mind (ToM) are considered as a distinctive feature of schizophrenia. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have suggested that aberrant activity among the regions comprising the mentalizing network is related to observed ToM deficits. However, the white matter structures underlying the ToM functional network in schizophrenia remain unclear. To investigate the relationship between white matter integrity and ToM impairment, 35 patients with first-episode psychosis (FEP) and 29 matched healthy controls (HCs) underwent diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS), fractional anisotropy (FA) values of the two regions of interest (ROI)–the cingulum and superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF)–were acquired, and correlational analysis with ToM task scores was performed. Among the patients with FEP, ToM strange story scores were positively correlated with the FA values of the left cingulum and left SLF. There was no significant correlation between FA and ToM task scores in HCs. These results suggest that the left cingulum and SLF constitute a possible neural basis for ToM deficits in schizophrenia. Our study is the first to demonstrate the white matter connectivity underlying the mentalizing network, as well as its relation to ToM ability in patients with FEP.

Introduction

Social cognition abnormalities are core features of schizophrenia (1, 2). Among many aspects of social cognition, deficits in theory of mind (ToM) are consistently reported in patients with schizophrenia across different stages of the disease (35). ToM is the mentalizing capacity to infer others' thoughts, intentions, beliefs, and emotions (6). Compared to its severity in other psychiatric disorders, ToM impairment is more severe in schizophrenia (7) and is strongly associated with psychopathology, neurocognitive function, and general functioning (1, 711). The impairment has trait-like characteristics that precede the onset of illness and persist across disease progression, even after remission (1214). Moreover, ToM deficits have been reported as a possible predictor of illness onset among individuals with high-risk psychosis (15). Taken together, these observations suggest the significance of ToM in understanding the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.

Over the last few decades, the neural correlates of ToM have been largely investigated using the functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) approach (16, 17). According to studies in healthy subjects, the mentalizing network, including the medial pre-frontal cortex (mPFC), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), and precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), is activated during the performance of various ToM tasks, regardless of task modality (2, 1720). In schizophrenia, the aberrant activities of the mPFC, TPJ, and precuneus/PCC within the mentalizing network were related to ToM deficits (2126).

To specify the direct structural connections among the abovementioned ToM regions, white matter studies are necessary. However, there are insufficient number of studies, despite the crucial roles of white matter structures in connecting distal cortical areas (17). Particularly in schizophrenia, only a few studies have attempted to investigate the relationship between white matter and social cognition, such as face perception (27), emotion attribution (28), empathy (29), and social relationships (30). Structural abnormalities that may cause disrupted neural activations and ToM impairment in schizophrenia have not yet been studied.

To explore the white matter neural correlates of ToM deficits in schizophrenia, the cingulum and superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) were selected as the white matter regions of interest (ROIs). According to the previous fMRI studies in schizophrenia and DTI literature from healthy individuals and those with other diseases, the two ROIs connect the crucial nodes of mentalizing network as the cingulum passes mPFC and precuneus/PCC and SLF passes through the PFC and TPJ (17, 3133).

The aim of this study was to investigate the association between ToM deficits and the cingulum and the SLF. With respect to ToM performance, it was hypothesized that patients with FEP would have decreased ToM abilities compared to those of HCs. Additionally, considering previous findings, impaired ToM abilities in FEP patients were hypothesized to be related to the FA reduction in the cingulum and SLF.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-five patients with FEP and 29 HCs participated in the study. Age, sex, and handedness were matched between the groups. All participants were part of a prospective cohort study recruited from the psychosis clinic at Seoul National University Hospital. Past and current psychotic symptoms of the patients were evaluated using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (34), and a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID-I) was administered. Additionally, Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) scores were collected. The inclusion criteria for FEP patients were being aged between 15 and 37 years old with a brief psychotic disorder, schizophreniform disorder, schizophrenia, or schizoaffective disorder. The duration of the illness of all FEP individuals was less than a year. Individuals were excluded from the HC group if they had a past or current SCID-I Non-patient Edition (SCID-NP) axis I diagnosis and any first- to third-degree biological relatives with a psychiatric disorder. The exclusion criteria for both groups were substance abuse, medical illness that could cause psychiatric symptoms, intellectual disability (intelligence quotient [IQ] <70), neurological disorders, or previous head injury. The study procedures were explained in detail to all participants, who then provided written informed consent. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University Hospital.

Behavioral Measures

ToM was assessed with the short form of two verbal ToM tasks: the false belief and strange story tasks (3538). All tasks were translated into Korean by psychiatrists and clinical psychologists, taking cultural backgrounds into accounts (39). The false belief task consisted of first-order (35) and second-order (36) tasks. The first-order task was used to evaluate whether the subject recognized a character's false belief about reality. The second-order task questioned a character's understanding of the other character's mental state. Each task is comprised of a short vignette with a picture and two questions: one for the comprehension test and the other to assess the subjects' capacity to infer the character's thoughts (justification question). The maximum total score of the false belief task was 12 points.

The strange story task (38) consisted of eight vignettes, each accompanied by a picture and two questions: one for comprehension test and the other to measure subjects' cognitive capacity to infer the character's mental state and emotion in complex naturalistic situations. The task included two examples for each of the four types of stories: double bluff, white lie, persuasion, and misunderstanding. The maximum score of the story task was 26.

Image Acquisition and DTI Pre-processing

All participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging scanning on a 3T scanner (MAGNETOM Trio Tim Syngo MR B17, 12 channel head coil, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) at Seoul National University Hospital. Diffusion tensor images were acquired via echo-planar imaging with the following parameters: TR 11400 ms, TE 88 ms, matrix 128 × 128, FOV 240 mm and a voxel size of 1.9 × 1.9 × 3.5. Diffusion-sensitizing gradient echo encoding was applied in 64 directions using a diffusion-weighting factor b of 1,000 s/mm2. One volume was acquired with b factor of 0 s/mm2 (without gradient).

The diffusion images were pre-processed via three steps with the FSL software package (version 5.0.10; https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). First, the eddy-current correction was applied to correct distortions and subject movements. Then, the skull was removed by the brain extraction tool (BET). After the BET process, raw brain images underwent visual inspection, and one healthy control was excluded because the dorsal surface of the brain was not covered in the MRI. As the final step, DTIFIT was applied to fit the diffusion tensor model, and individual FA values were obtained.

Region of Interest

To test the structural connectivity among the mentalizing network, two white matter tracts were selected as the regions of interest (ROI). The tracts included the left and right cingulum, which pass through the PFC and precuneus, and the left and right SLF, which pass through the mPFC and TPJ. The ROI masks were obtained from Johns Hopkins University ICBM-DTI-81 white-matter labels atlas (4042) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Region of interest (ROI) masks obtained from Johns Hopkins University ICBM-DTI-81 white-matter labels atlas overlaid on the white matter skeleton. (A) Cingulum (Yellow). (B) Superior longitudinal fasciculus (blue).

DTI Processing

Voxel-wise statistical analysis was performed using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) in FSL (43, 44). First, a brain mask was generated as a pre-processing step. Then, all subjects' FA images were aligned into a 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 space via FMRIB's Non-linear Image Registration Tool (FNIRT). The aligned images were all merged into a single 4D image file, and the mean FA image was created. A 4D image of the FA skeleton was generated from the mean FA with a threshold of 0.2.

Voxel-wise significant differences between the FEP and HC were investigated using the randomize tool in TBSS. Before processing, age, sex, and handedness were demeaned and fed into the design matrix and contrast file as covariates. The randomize was carried out with 5,000 permutations and threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE). The left and right cingulum and SLF masks were used as ROI masks. The threshold for significance was p < 0.05.

Statistical Analysis

The age, sex, and handedness of the final set of subjects were tested to determine whether the variables matched between the groups. The normality of the ToM task scores (e.g., false belief and strange story) was verified, and the scores were compared between the groups via the Mann-Whitney test.

To explore the correlation between ToM task results and the white matter integrity of ROIs, the individual mean FA of each ROI was acquired from 3D individual skeleton images. The individual images were obtained by splitting the 4D skeleton image, which was created from TBSS analysis. Correlation analyses were performed for each mean FA of the cingulum and SLF for both the left and right sides with the false belief and strange story. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS, version 25 (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.).

Results

Demographic Data

The demographic data of the subjects are presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences in the sex ratio, age, handedness, IQ, and education year between the FEP and HC groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the subjects.

Theory of Mind Task Scores

The Mann-Whitney test revealed significant group differences in the two ToM task scores (Table 2). FEP patients exhibited significantly worse performance than HCs in the false belief task (z = −3.506, p < 0.001) and strange story task (z = −4.049, p < 0.001). The ToM task results are presented in Figure 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Theory of mind task results.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Individual scores of theory of mind tasks (false belief task, strange task) tested using Mann-Whitney. (A) False belief scores of FEP patients and HCs (Z = −3.506, p < 0.001). (B) Strange story scores of FEP patients and HCs (Z = −4.049, p < 0.001). FE, first-episode psychosis; HC, healthy control. ***P ≤ 0.001.

TBSS Data and Correlations

TBSS analysis showed no significant voxel-wise difference in any of the ROI regions between FEP and HC groups. To explore the relationship between ToM and white matter integrity, correlational analyses were performed. The results, presented in Figure 3, showed a significant positive correlation between the FA value of the left cingulum and strange story task scores in FEP patients (r = 0.35, p = 0.039). Also, a significant positive correlation was observed between the FA value of the left SLF and the scores of the strange story task (r = 0.374, p = 0.027) in FEP patients. No correlation was found between the FA values and false belief scores in FEP patients. There was no significant correlation among the FA values and any ToM task in HCs.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlations between the mean fractional anisotropy (FA) values and the strange story task score in first-episode psychosis (FEP) patients. (A) The mean FA of the left cingulum and the strange story score (r = 0.350; p = 0.039). (B) The mean FA of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) and the strange story score (r = 0.374; p = 0.027).

Discussion

The present study was designed to explore the structural basis of theory of mind (ToM) deficits in schizophrenia. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first attempt to demonstrate the relationship between white matter integrity and ToM in first-episode psychosis (FEP). The results of the false belief task and strange story task showed that the patients with FEP had impaired ToM abilities. The correlation analysis revealed positive associations between the integrity of left ROIs and ToM deficits in FEP patients. However, this correlation was not observed in the healthy controls (HCs). These findings underscore the crucial roles of the left cingulum and left SLF as the structural basis of impaired ToM abilities in FEP.

ToM Abilities in FEP

The ToM task results were consistent with previous studies that showed decreased ToM performance in FEP patients (5, 45, 46). In this study, two verbal ToM tasks–the false belief task and strange story task–were conducted. The false belief task is the most heavily researched ToM task and is used to assess participants' ability to understand others' mental states, such as thoughts and intentions. The strange story task, invented to measure higher-order ToM, is employed to evaluate the ability to infer not only the thoughts or intentions but also the emotions of others in complex naturalistic situations (38). In the false belief task, FEP patients scored significantly lower than healthy controls, suggesting that the patients have an impaired ability to recognize others' mental states. Similar to the false belief results, the strange story scores of FEP patients were also significantly lower than those of HCs, suggesting the difficulties patients have in inferring others' mental states and emotions in complex situations. These results are in line with previous findings showing ToM deficits as a distinctive feature of schizophrenia, which is found not only in chronic patients but also in the early course of the disease and even in remission (12, 14, 47). As previous meta-analysis studies have suggested, these ToM impairments may be related to the functional outcome of the patients and could be able to predict prognosis (1, 9, 11).

White Matter Integrity and ToM Abilities

The two ROIs–the cingulum and SLF–were selected based on the atlas-listed white matter tracts that are reported to connect the nodes of the mentalizing network; the mPFC, TPJ, and precuneus/PCC. Our results suggest that the integrity of these white matter ROIs are positively correlated with the strange story task performance in FEP. The correlations were maintained in significant level (p < 0.05) even after controlling age and olanzapine equivalent antipsychotic doses (Supplementary Tables 3, 5). This finding is in line with recent DTI studies in healthy children and other patient groups reporting that ToM abilities were associated with the integrity of the cingulum and SLF (3133, 48).

The cingulum is an association fiber tract that starts from the mPFC and passes through the PCC and precuneus. More precisely, a probabilistic tractography study has revealed that among the listed tracts in the atlas, the cingulum overlaps with 62.59% of the dorsomedial pre-frontal cortex (dmPFC)–PCC fibers and 92.01% of the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex (vmPFC)–PCC fibers (17). The cingulum is known to be involved in attention, memory, and emotional processing (49, 50) and has been postulated as a major white matter tract comprising the mentalizing network (31). In schizophrenia, decreased FA in the cingulum has been reported in both chronic and FEP patients (5155) and has been linked to both positive and negative symptoms and decreased neurocognition (56, 57).

The SLF is a large association fiber bundle that connects the parietal, occipital and temporal lobes with the frontal cortex (58, 59). The SLF is a core structure involved in attention, memory, language, and emotions (6063). According to a probabilistic tractography study, 45% of fibers between the dmPFC and TPJ overlap with the SLF (17). Similar to the cingulum, abnormalities in the SLF have been observed in schizophrenia (6466) and in association with symptoms and neurocognition (67, 68). Along with our results, the abovementioned observations provide converging evidence that the cingulum and SLF are key structures underlying the symptoms and both social and non-social cognitions of schizophrenia.

In this study, only the strange story task scores that reflect higher order ToM were correlated with white matter integrity in FEP patients. According to previous research, complex and higher-order ToM continues to mature until adulthood (47, 69, 70). Several imaging studies have demonstrated neural activations and brain structures related to higher-order ToM change throughout adolescence (7173). This evidence suggests that the correlation between strange story scores and white matter may reflect an abnormal developmental trajectory of higher-order ToM and related white matter structures in schizophrenia. These findings corroborate the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia (74, 75) and could offer insight into the pathophysiology of the disease, especially focusing on the prodromal stage and early phase.

In contrast to the results of FEP patients, no correlation between the ToM task scores and WM integrities were found in HCs. There can be several reasons for this. First, the homogeneity of the HC group could have led to smaller intragroup variations of the task scores. It is known that age and education level are associated with theory of mind ability (76) but those factors were strictly controlled in this study. Second, it is possible that HCs are less dependent on the direct white matter pathways connecting the mentalizing network regions. Instead, they could use various direct and indirect structural connections among distal brain regions to subserve ToM ability since their neural structures are relatively intact compared to schizophrenia patients. Conversely, schizophrenia patients may utilize large white matter bundles such as cingulum and SLF for ToM process due to the extensive neural degeneration. To specify the association of white matter structures and ToM ability in healthy participants, further research is needed with other ToM task modalities, such as “reading the mind in the eyes” test and subjects with different conditions.

Another finding of the study is that only the left ROIs showed correlation with the ToM task scores in FEP patients. This result matches with the studies of children with traumatic brain injury and patients with surgical resection for diffuse low-grade glioma that reported associations between the left cingulum and ToM abilities (33, 77). These observations suggest that the key white matter structures underlying ToM deficits in FEP may be lateralized to the left hemisphere. However, several studies have indicated that the bilateral or right cingulum is related to ToM abilities (31, 78). Such inconsistency is also evident in findings related to the SLF (32, 48, 7881).

Despite the inconsistent results, there are studies that have proposed the right SLF may play a crucial role in ToM abilities, while the left contributes mainly to language processing (78, 82). This argument is in contrast to our findings, which showed a relation between the left SLF and ToM ability in FEP patients. There are several possible explanations for this discrepancy. First, different subject groups may result in different outcomes. The “right social brain” argument was developed based on studies of healthy participants and brain damaged patients. However, multiple fMRI studies in schizophrenia and FEP patients have reported bilateral abnormalities in ToM networks (21, 83, 84). This observation suggests that the association between white matter and ToM in schizophrenia may be different than that in other groups.

In addition, the laterality difference between other subject groups and FEP patients may reflect Crows' lateralization hypotheses of schizophrenia (85). Numerous studies have indicated reduced or altered asymmetry in both brain functional connectivity and structures in schizophrenia patients (86, 87), and such alterations are also found in FEP patients (68, 85). These abnormalities in brain asymmetry and functional outcomes are related to developmental problems and the laterality changes in accordance with age (88). Therefore, factors such as the age of onset are closely related to abnormal brain asymmetry (89). Along with the variance in ToM task modalities and heterogeneity among FEP groups, altered or reduced brain asymmetry may have affected the results differently. Since this is the first attempt to investigate the specific white matter structure underlying ToM deficits in schizophrenia, future studies will be necessary to provide reliable evidence on the structural basis of ToM impairment.

Limitations

Several limitations must be taken into consideration when interpreting the present results. First, no significant differences in FA values between FEP patients and HCs were found in this study. Several DTI studies have reported that the arcuate fasciculus, SLF, and cingulum are intact in FEP patients (9092), while others have reported white matter alterations in the SLF and cingulum (54, 65). Such inconsistencies may reflect the heterogeneity of the FEP subjects. Symptoms, medications, age of onset, treatment intervention, and other factors may affect white matter changes differently. Additionally, if abnormal structures in FEP patients were located in small sub regions within the cingulum or SLF, the ROI-based TBSS approach would not be able to detect these abnormalities. Since the cingulum and SLF are widely distributed fiber tracts connecting various distal brain regions, averaging the FA values across the whole ROI risks losing some valuable information. To address these issues and to define precise fiber tracts related to impaired ToM in FEP, a probabilistic tractography analysis while controlling the covariates would be necessary in future studies. Second, unlike previous studies the FEP patients in this study were on antipsychotics at the time of scanning and ToM measurements. Antipsychotics are known to be associated with structural alteration in brain (93). However, there was no correlation between olanzapine equivalent antipsychotic doses of the FEP patients and their FA values of white matter ROIs in this study (Supplementary Table 2). Also, the additional partial correlational analysis revealed that the positive correlations between higher order ToM and left cingulum and left SLF were maintained in significant level (p < 0.05) even after controlling the effect of antipsychotics. Although, given the previous studies that antipsychotics are associated with white matter alteration (94), it may be beneficial to investigate the relationship between white matter and ToM in drug-naïve FEP patients in future studies.

Conclusion

Our study was the first to demonstrate the association between FA values in two white matter tracts, the cingulum and the SLF, and ToM ability in FEP patients. This white matter study using DTI methods extends our insight into the neural basis of ToM and suggests that the left cingulum and SLF are vital structures underlying the impairment of ToM in schizophrenia.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Academic Affairs Institutional Review Board Seoul National University Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

NK: conceptualization, methodology, software, validation, formal analysis, investigation, data curation, writing–original draft, writing–review and editing, and visualization. TL: conceptualization, methodology, validation, writing–review and editing, and supervision. WH: conceptualization, methodology, and writing–review and editing. YK: software and writing–review and editing. SK: validation, investigation, and writing–review and editing. S-YM: investigation and writing–review and editing. SL: investigation and writing–review and editing. SO: investigation and writing–review and editing. JK: conceptualization, writing–review and editing, resources, supervision, project administration, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Brain Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning (Grant No. 2017M3C7A1029610).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.617683/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:573–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:620–31. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Frith CD, Corcoran R. Exploring 'theory of mind' in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med. (1996) 26:521–30. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700035601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E, Hox J, van Engeland H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2007) 191:5–13. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bora E, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. (2013) 144:31–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Frith CD, Frith U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron. (2006) 50:531–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. (2009) 109:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Roncone R, Falloon IR, Mazza M, De Risio A, Pollice R, Necozione S, et al. Is theory of mind in schizophrenia more strongly associated with clinical and social functioning than with neurocognitive deficits? Psychopathology. (2002) 35:280–8. doi: 10.1159/000067062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Champagne-Lavau M, Charest A, Anselmo K, Rodriguez JP, Blouin G. Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: the role of cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 200:184–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Cotter J, Granger K, Backx R, Hobbs M, Looi CY, Barnett JH. Social cognitive dysfunction as a clinical marker: a systematic review of meta-analyses across 30 clinical conditions. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 84:92–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Catalan A, Angosto V, Diaz A, Martinez N, Guede D, Pereda M, et al. The relationship between theory of mind deficits and neurocognition in first episode-psychosis. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 268:361–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.06.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Inoue Y, Yamada K, Hirano M, Shinohara M, Tamaoki T, Iguchi H, et al. Impairment of theory of mind in patients in remission following first episode of schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2006) 256:326–8. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0608-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Green MF, Bearden CE, Cannon TD, Fiske AP, Hellemann GS, Horan WP, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia, part 1: performance across phase of illness. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:854–64. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Lee TY, Hong SB, Shin NY, Kwon JS. Social cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. (2015) 164:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kim HS, Shin NY, Jang JH, Kim E, Shim G, Park HY, et al. Social cognition and neurocognition as predictors of conversion to psychosis in individuals at ultra-high risk. Schizophr Res. (2011) 130:170–5. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kronbichler L, Tschernegg M, Martin AI, Schurz M, Kronbichler M. Abnormal brain activation during theory of mind tasks in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. (2017) 43:1240–50. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wang Y, Metoki A, Alm KH, Olson IR. White matter pathways and social cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 90:350–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ. Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:2313–35. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 42:9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mar RA. The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu Rev Psychol. (2011) 62:103–34. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Lee J, Quintana J, Nori P, Green MF. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: exploring neural mechanisms of belief attribution. Soc Neurosci. (2011) 6:569–81. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.620774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. de Achával D, Villarreal MF, Costanzo EY, Douer J, Castro MN, Mora MC, et al. Decreased activity in right-hemisphere structures involved in social cognition in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2012) 134:171–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Das P, Lagopoulos J, Coulston CM, Henderson AF, Malhi GS. Mentalizing impairment in schizophrenia: a functional MRI study. Schizophr Res. (2012) 134:158–64. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Eack SM, Wojtalik JA, Newhill CE, Keshavan MS, Phillips ML. Prefrontal cortical dysfunction during visual perspective-taking in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2013) 150:491–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Dodell-Feder D, Tully LM, Lincoln SH, Hooker CI. The neural basis of theory of mind and its relationship to social functioning and social anhedonia in individuals with schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. (2014) 4:154–63. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Bartholomeusz CF, Ganella EP, Whittle S, Allott K, Thompson A, Abu-Akel A, et al. An fMRI study of theory of mind in individuals with first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2018) 281:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Zhao X, Sui Y, Yao J, Lv Y, Zhang X, Jin Z, et al. Reduced white matter integrity and facial emotion perception in never-medicated patients with first-episode schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 77:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Miyata J, Yamada M, Namiki C, Hirao K, Saze T, Fujiwara H, et al. Reduced white matter integrity as a neural correlate of social cognition deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2010) 119:232–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fujino J, Takahashi H, Miyata J, Sugihara G, Kubota M, Sasamoto A, et al. Impaired empathic abilities and reduced white matter integrity in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2014) 48:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Saito Y, Kubicki M, Koerte I, Otsuka T, Rathi Y, Pasternak O, et al. Impaired white matter connectivity between regions containing mirror neurons, and relationship to negative symptoms and social cognition, in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Brain Imaging Behav. (2018) 12:229–37. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9685-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Yordanova YN, Duffau H, Herbet G. Neural pathways subserving face-based mentalizing. Brain Struct Funct. (2017) 222:3087–105. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1388-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Jalbrzikowski M, Villalon-Reina JE, Karlsgodt KH, Senturk D, Chow C, Thompson PM, et al. Altered white matter microstructure is associated with social cognition and psychotic symptoms in 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome. Front Behav Neurosci. (2014) 8:393. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Levin HS, Wilde EA, Hanten G, Li X, Chu ZD, Vasquez AC, et al. Mental state attributions and diffusion tensor imaging after traumatic brain injury in children. Dev Neuropsychol. (2011) 36:273–87. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition. (1983) 13:103–28. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Perner J, Wimmer H. “John thinks that Mary thinks that.” attribution of 2nd-order beliefs by 5-year-old to 10-year-old children. J Exp Child Psychol. (1985) 39:437–71. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Happé F, Brownell H, Winner E. Acquired 'theory of mind' impairments following stroke. Cognition. (1999) 70:211–40. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Happé FG. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord. (1994) 24:129–54. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Chung YS, Kang DH, Shin NY, Yoo SY, Kwon JS. Deficit of theory of mind in individuals at ultra-high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2008) 99:111–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Mori S, Wakana S, Van Zijl PC, Nagae-Poetscher LM. MRI Atlas of Human White Matter. Elsevier (2005).

Google Scholar

41. Hua K, Zhang J, Wakana S, Jiang H, Li X, Reich DS, et al. Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tract-specific quantification. Neuroimage. (2008) 39:336–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Wakana S, Caprihan A, Panzenboeck MM, Fallon JH, Perry M, Gollub RL, et al. Reproducibility of quantitative tractography methods applied to cerebral white matter. Neuroimage. (2007) 36:630–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay CE, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. (2006) 31:1487–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. (2004) 23(Suppl. 1):S208–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Song MJ, Choi HI, Jang SK, Lee SH, Ikezawa S, Choi KH. Theory of mind in Koreans with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 229:420–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lindgren M, Torniainen-Holm M, Heiskanen I, Voutilainen G, Pulkkinen U, Mehtala T, et al. Theory of mind in a first-episode psychosis population using the Hinting Task. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 263:185–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Stanford AD, Messinger J, Malaspina D, Corcoran CM. Theory of Mind in patients at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2011) 131:11–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Grosse Wiesmann C, Schreiber J, Singer T, Steinbeis N, Friederici AD. White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of Theory of Mind in early childhood. Nat Commun. (2017) 8:14692. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Catani M, Dell'acqua F, Thiebaut de Schotten M. A revised limbic system model for memory, emotion and behaviour. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37:1724–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Catani M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex. (2008) 44:1105–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kubicki M, Westin CF, Nestor PG, Wible CG, Frumin M, Maier SE, et al. Cingulate fasciculus integrity disruption in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance diffusion tensor imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. (2003) 54:1171–80. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00419-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Sun Z, Wang F, Cui L, Breeze J, Du X, Wang X, et al. Abnormal anterior cingulum in patients with schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroreport. (2003) 14:1833–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200310060-00015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Wang F, Sun Z, Cui L, Du X, Wang X, Zhang H, et al. Anterior cingulum abnormalities in male patients with schizophrenia determined through diffusion tensor imaging. Am J Psychiatry. (2004) 161:573–5. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Federspiel A, Begre S, Kiefer C, Schroth G, Strik WK, Dierks T. Alterations of white matter connectivity in first episode schizophrenia. Neurobiol Dis. (2006) 22:702–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2006.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Lee SH, Kubicki M, Asami T, Seidman LJ, Goldstein JM, Mesholam-Gately RI, et al. Extensive white matter abnormalities in patients with first-episode schizophrenia: a Diffusion Tensor Iimaging (DTI) study. Schizophr Res. (2013) 143:231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Fujiwara H, Namiki C, Hirao K, Miyata J, Shimizu M, Fukuyama H, et al. Anterior and posterior cingulum abnormalities and their association with psychopathology in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Schizophr Res. (2007) 95:215–22. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.05.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Whitford TJ, Lee SW, Oh JS, de Luis-Garcia R, Savadjiev P, Alvarado JL, et al. Localized abnormalities in the cingulum bundle in patients with schizophrenia: a Diffusion Tensor tractography study. Neuroimage Clin. (2014) 5:93–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Schmahmann JD, Smith EE, Eichler FS, Filley CM. Cerebral white matter: neuroanatomy, clinical neurology, and neurobehavioral correlates. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2008) 1142:266–309. doi: 10.1196/annals.1444.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kamali A, Flanders AE, Brody J, Hunter JV, Hasan KM. Tracing superior longitudinal fasciculus connectivity in the human brain using high resolution diffusion tensor tractography. Brain Struct Funct. (2014) 219:269–81. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0498-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Klarborg B, Skak Madsen K, Vestergaard M, Skimminge A, Jernigan TL, Baare WF. Sustained attention is associated with right superior longitudinal fasciculus and superior parietal white matter microstructure in children. Hum Brain Mapp. (2013) 34:3216–32. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Vestergaard M, Madsen KS, Baare WF, Skimminge A, Ejersbo LR, Ramsoy TZ, et al. White matter microstructure in superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with spatial working memory performance in children. J Cogn Neurosci. (2011) 23:2135–46. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Madhavan KM, McQueeny T, Howe SR, Shear P, Szaflarski J. Superior longitudinal fasciculus and language functioning in healthy aging. Brain Res. (2014) 1562:11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Multani N, Galantucci S, Wilson SM, Shany-Ur T, Poorzand P, Growdon ME, et al. Emotion detection deficits and changes in personality traits linked to loss of white matter integrity in primary progressive aphasia. Neuroimage Clin. (2017) 16:447–54. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Buchsbaum MS, Schoenknecht P, Torosjan Y, Newmark R, Chu KW, Mitelman S, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of frontal lobe white matter tracts in schizophrenia. Ann Gen Psychiatry. (2006) 5:19. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-5-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Szeszko PR, Robinson DG, Ashtari M, Vogel J, Betensky J, Sevy S, et al. Clinical and neuropsychological correlates of white matter abnormalities in recent onset schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2008) 33:976–84. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Knöchel C, O'Dwyer L, Alves G, Reinke B, Magerkurth J, Rotarska-Jagiela A, et al. Association between white matter fiber integrity and subclinical psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia patients and unaffected relatives. Schizophr Res. (2012) 140:129–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Karlsgodt KH, van Erp TG, Poldrack RA, Bearden CE, Nuechterlein KH, Cannon TD. Diffusion tensor imaging of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and working memory in recent-onset schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 63:512–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Szeszko PR, Tan ET, Ulug AM, Kingsley PB, Gallego JA, Rhindress K, et al. Investigation of superior longitudinal fasciculus fiber complexity in recent onset psychosis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 81:114–21. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. O'Hare AE, Bremner L, Nash M, Happe F, Pettigrew LM. A clinical assessment tool for advanced theory of mind performance in 5 to 12 year olds. J Autism Dev Disord. (2009) 39:916–28. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0699-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Kaland N, Callesen K, Moller-Nielsen A, Mortensen EL, Smith L. Performance of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism on advanced theory of mind tasks. J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:1112–23. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0496-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, Dapretto M. Developmental changes in the neural basis of interpreting communicative intent. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2006) 1:107–21. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Mori T, Matsuda H, Komaki G. Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2007) 61:355–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01687.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:267–77. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Murray RM, Lewis SW. Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder? Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). (1987) 295:681–2. doi: 10.1136/bmj.295.6600.681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Owen MJ, O'Donovan MC. Schizophrenia and the neurodevelopmental continuum:evidence from genomics. World Psychiatry. (2017) 16:227–35. doi: 10.1002/wps.20440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Franco MG, Smith PK. Theory of mind, old age, and educational opportunity in Colombia. Int J Aging Hum Dev. (2013) 77:107–25. doi: 10.2190/AG.77.2.b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Herbet G, Lafargue G, Moritz-Gasser S, Menjot de Champfleur N, Costi E, Bonnetblanc F, et al. A disconnection account of subjective empathy impairments in diffuse low-grade glioma patients. Neuropsychologia. (2015) 70:165–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Herbet G, Lafargue G, Bonnetblanc F, Moritz-Gasser S, Menjot de Champfleur N, Duffau H. Inferring a dual-stream model of mentalizing from associative white matter fibres disconnection. Brain. (2014) 137(Pt 3):944–59. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Cabinio M, Rossetto F, Blasi V, Savazzi F, Castelli I, Massaro D, et al. Mind-reading ability and structural connectivity changes in aging. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:1808. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Kana RK, Libero LE, Hu CP, Deshpande HD, Colburn JS. Functional brain networks and white matter underlying theory-of-mind in autism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2014) 9:98–105. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Herbet G, Lafargue G, Moritz-Gasser S, Bonnetblanc F, Duffau H. Interfering with the neural activity of mirror-related frontal areas impairs mentalistic inferences. Brain Struct Funct. (2015) 220:2159–69. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0777-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Nagae LM, Zarnow DM, Blaskey L, Dell J, Khan SY, Qasmieh S, et al. Elevated mean diffusivity in the left hemisphere superior longitudinal fasciculus in autism spectrum disorders increases with more profound language impairment. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. (2012) 33:1720–5. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Brüne M, Lissek S, Fuchs N, Witthaus H, Peters S, Nicolas V, et al. An fMRI study of theory of mind in schizophrenic patients with “passivity” symptoms. Neuropsychologia. (2008) 46:1992–2001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Beauchamp MS. The social mysteries of the superior temporal sulcus. Trends Cogn Sci. (2015) 19:489–90. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Crow TJ, Ball J, Bloom SR, Brown R, Bruton CJ, Colter N, et al. Schizophrenia as an anomaly of development of cerebral asymmetry. A postmortem study and a proposal concerning the genetic basis of the disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1989) 46:1145–50. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810120087013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Mitchell RL, Crow TJ. Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere? Brain. (2005) 128(Pt 5):963–78. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Ribolsi M, Daskalakis ZJ, Siracusano A, Koch G. Abnormal asymmetry of brain connectivity in schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:1010. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Crow TJ, Colter N, Frith CD, Johnstone EC, Owens DG. Developmental arrest of cerebral asymmetries in early onset schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (1989) 29:247–53. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90053-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Aso M, Kurachi M, Suzuki M, Yuasa S, Matsui M, Saitoh O. Asymmetry of the ventricle and age at the onset of schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1995) 245:142–4. doi: 10.1007/BF02193086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Peters BD, de Haan L, Dekker N, Blaas J, Becker HE, Dingemans PM, et al. White matter fibertracking in first-episode schizophrenia, schizoaffective patients and subjects at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Neuropsychobiology. (2008) 58:19–28. doi: 10.1159/000154476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Kawashima T, Nakamura M, Bouix S, Kubicki M, Salisbury DF, Westin CF, et al. Uncinate fasciculus abnormalities in recent onset schizophrenia and affective psychosis: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Schizophr Res. (2009) 110:119–26. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Luck D, Buchy L, Czechowska Y, Bodnar M, Pike GB, Campbell JS, et al. Fronto-temporal disconnectivity and clinical short-term outcome in first episode psychosis: a DTI-tractography study. J Psychiatr Res. (2011) 45:369–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Ziebell S, Pierson R, Magnotta V. Long-term antipsychotic treatment and brain volumes: a longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2011) 68:128–37. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Bartzokis G, Lu PH, Nuechterlein KH, Gitlin M, Doi C, Edwards N, et al. Differential effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on brain myelination in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2007) 93:13–22. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social cognition, theory of mind, schizophrenia, first episode psychosis, DTI, TBSS, white matter

Citation: Kim NS, Lee TY, Hwang WJ, Kwak YB, Kim S, Moon S-Y, Lho SK, Oh S and Kwon JS (2021) White Matter Correlates of Theory of Mind in Patients With First-Episode Psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 12:617683. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.617683

Received: 15 October 2020; Accepted: 08 February 2021;
Published: 05 March 2021.

Edited by:

Luca De Peri, Cantonal Sociopsychological Organization, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Xiaohua Liu, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China
Teresa Sanchez-Gutierrez, Universidad Internacional De La Rioja, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Kim, Lee, Hwang, Kwak, Kim, Moon, Lho, Oh and Kwon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jun Soo Kwon, kwonjs@snu.ac.kr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.